3 4
differences in behavior by the children in the ownership and
control conditions. However, their follow-up questions did
show that possession changed the children's perceptions of
ownership. Eighty-five percent of the children in the
ownership condition said they did not know who owned the toy
and 5% said it was the experimenter's. Whereas, 40% of the
control children said they did not know who owned the toy
and 40% said it belonged to the experimenter. Even
possession of an object by preschool children for short
periods in an institutional setting was sufficient to
decrease the perceived probability that the object belonged
to the institution.
The prominence of the Social Defensive criteria,
however, should not overshadow the finding that ownership
also seems to entail a characteristic of Familiarity.
Further, Utility was valued most similarly with Familiarity,
and these two clustered as Favored Objects criteria in one
analysis and as Regular Acquisition criteria in another.
Apparently, subjects had a tendency to list items they were
familiar with and used routinely. Again, this may reflect
the contact hypothesis (Zajonc, 1968). But much earlier,
the philosopher Hume (1739/1962) recognized the
Familiarity-Utility pairing and its relevance as a major
criterion of ownership:
such is the effect of custom that it not only
reconciles us to anything that we have long enjoyed,