wr
occupation and defense of territory seem in many respects analogous to the ownership. of
property. Edney (1974) examined 14 definitions of territoriality and found the component
concepts to be space, defense, possession, identity, markers, personalization, control, and
exclusive use. These could easily be a listing of the component concepts of definitions of
property. Altman’s (1970) definition was most inclusive and clearly subsumes the owning of
property:
Human territoriality encompasses temporarily durable preventive and reactive behaviors
including perceptions, use and defense of places, people, objects, and ideas by means of
verbal, self-marker, and environmental prop behaviors in response to the actual or implied
presence of others and in response to properties of the environment, and is geared to
satisfy certain primary and secondary motivational states of individuals and groups.
(Altman, 1970, p. 8)
Here, territorial control of objects and ideas would seem little different from proprietary control
of objects and ideas.
The definition of territoriality by Sack (1983), a geographer, moves the purpose of
territoriality from control of territory to control of objects, people and their relations:
At this point let me define what | mean by territoriality explicitly: the attempt by an
individual or group (x) to influence, affect, or control objects, people, and relationships (vy)
by delimiting and asserting control over a geographic area. (Sack, 1983, p. 56)
Whereas Brown (1965) emphasized the high cognitive demands of a dominance hierarchy
(identifying one’s own species, then sex within the species, then individual superiors and
subordinates), Sack (1983; 1986) emphasized the cognitive efficiencies of territoriality: it avoids
enumeration of the possessions and activities to be controlled; it is easy to communicate by
boundary markers and at the same time efficiently enforces control at the boundary; it reifies
power and makes it tangible; it displaces attention from interpersonal power differences and
makes them impersonal; it controls complex activities within territories; it reifies activities within
a space and allows attributions of agency and personality to a space.
Animal ethologists and sociobiologists have traditionally linked hierarchical dominance
and territoriality as correlative systems of intraspecies control (e.g. Wilson, 1975). Many
psychological studies have shown a correlation between dominance and territoriality. Most of
these have been studies of captive male populations. Edney (1974) has reviewed these studies,
giving an appropriate caution against generalizing from special sub-populations of males
confined in close, supervised settings.
Blood and Livant (1957) reported that young boys in a summer camp spaced their beds
according to friendships while boys over 10 spaced them according to dominance hierarchy.