9
has been most significant on moral development research (e.g. Kohlberg, 1969), especially on
possessiveness and altruism (e.g. Berti, Bombi & Lis, 1982; Furby, 1976; 1978a,b; Moessinger,
1975).
Furby (1978a,b) completed a Piagetian interview survey of property behaviors, including
sharing, across age groups in three types of societies (U.S. urban, Israell urban, and Israeli
kibbutz). In the U.S, it appears that considerations about the welfare of others begin to appear
at about age 10 or 11. According to Furby (1978c), the interpretation that this is due to a
Piaget-staged, cognitive ability to take another's point of view (Flavell, 1968; Piaget, 1932) is
challenged by the lack of evidence of relationships of generosity with role-taking, cognitive
ability, or vicarious experience (e.g. Emler & Rushton, 1974; Rushton & Weiner, 1975). Citing
Isaacs’ (1949/1967) psychodynamic explanation of children’s sharing behavior and Krebs’ (1970)
review conclusion that dependency elicits altruism, Furby (1978c) alternatively suggests that
altruistic behavior may affirm power, control and effectance in the world and thus in those
respects, would have similar motivational origins as possessiveness. Furby (1980) has also
reviewed evidence that there may be a developmental sequence of dominance relationships
first, followed by affiliation relationships, and later altruism.
Other child development studies have emphasized the importance of control motivations
to the expression of altruistic behavior. Merei (1949) identified primary grade school children
who had previously displayed leadership, introduced them into established play groups, and
observed their strategies to regain power. One strategy was to assume ownership of important
resources in the playroom and to then give these to their original possessors. This resulted in
no changes in the children’s use of objects, but did re-establish the power of the leader.
Vanderbilt (1971) differentiated kindergarten children who were socially active from those
who were passive, hypothesizing that actives would be more generous. Contrary to
expectations, actives kept more candy than they gave away, despite the fact that actives were
more aware of sharing as a socialization norm at home, that they made more references to
sharing in projective stories, and that they did more actual sharing in play. Actives also had
higher teacher ratings on traits of control and drive-regulation. Vanderbiit (1971) concluded that
actives have greater ego strength and that they are socially more adaptive and able to actin
their own or others’ interests as conditions dictate.