—-
Hobhouse's influence on the cross-cultural psychological study of ownership continued,
largely through the work of Ginsberg, his co-author and junior colleague at the London School
of Economics. Ginsberg supervised the first thesis on the psychology of property, which was
done by Ernest Beaglehole in 1931. This work, published in 1932, reviewed the biological,
ethnographic and child development literature on ownership. Beaglehole (1932) noted that
random sampling was necessary if comparative research was to escape ideological bias and
he included probably the first random sample of cultures in the history of cross-cultural
research, sampling from the ethnographic codings of his mentors (Hobhouse, Wheeler &
Ginsberg, 1915). Although Beaglehole (1932) noted his intentions to do a more thorough
holocultural study of property, his career led off to ethnographic field research and he did not
return to the topic (Rudmin, in press). However, Beaglehole's academic heir in New Zealand is
Ritchie, and Ritchie’s student, Hills (1984), is today engaged in cross-cultural studies of
conceptualizations of land ownership.
Ginsberg himself continued with the psychology of property and was one of the
recognized experts of his day (e.g. Suttie, Ginsberg, Isaacs & Marshall, 1935). In a summary
statement reminiscent of Dugald Stewart, he argued that property cannot be explained by a
single, simple psychological principle. In this, he touched on all five of the traditional
psychological theories of ownership: 1) centering property on the sense of self, 2) repudiating
direct instincts of acquisition but still using the term “drive”, 3) adopting Bosanquet’'s (1895)
Aristotelian ethical theory of property for purposeful self-development, 4) replacing the
association of ideas with behaviorist and Piagetian cognitive equivalents, and 5) mentioning the
tendency to use property, which is a means, as a symbol for power, which is an end. He also
thoroughly linked property to interpersonal dominance. Ginsberg's statement merits a quote
at length:
...The interest in ownership is very complex and has its roots in several fundamental needs.
Things come to have ‘value’, either because they satisfy needs directly, or through a
process of ‘conditioning’ or assimilation. It is well known that objects originally indifferent
may come to be charged or infected with interest, by being linked with a train of events
culminating in satisfaction. In this way, habits of attachment may be formed in relation to
objects which may have no intrinsic or prima facie attractiveness. In a great many cases,
our interest in property consists just of such habits of attachment; in others, complicated
sentiments are involved. Objects connected directly or indirectly with the satisfaction of
important needs gather around them groups of emotional dispositions, including especially
the prospective and retrospective emotions of desire, hope, fear, anxiety, disappointment,
as well as pleasure in attainment, and joy in mastery. All the primary needs, sex, nutrition,
aesthetics and cognitive interests may serve as nuclei for the sentiments of property, aided
by the tendency to confuse means and ends. Constant or recurring needs are of special
importance, giving the objects to which they are attached an abiding value. These